I don’t use it flippantly simply because I know to do so can easily annoy people, and it’s not a case where that’s much of an impingement on my behaviour.
I feel this is quite decent of me as I advocate against blanket use or blanket belief in trigger warnings, and I say this, and why, whenever people are advocating them. I regularly revisit the state of research on them. There are theories both ways, but empirical evidence is so far one way.
The 2020 introduction to a typology study (trying to sort how warnings are presented, much like our own recent topic on intfiction) says:
More recent studies have experimentally investigated the impact of content warnings, especially in educational settings. A randomised study found participants with no trauma history (n = 133) who received warnings before reading passages with disturbing content reported more anxiety than those not receiving (n = 137) warnings, suggesting warnings can undermine emotional resilience [15]. The same authors replicated this finding with a college student sample (n = 462) [16], and also showed in a randomised study of trauma survivors (n = 451) that content warnings inadvertently reinforce the centrality of trauma experiences to identity [17]. A 2019 meta-analysis confirmed this finding that content warnings are associated with increased anxiety and negative mood [1]. Meta-analyses of a series of studies involving students and internet volunteers, with and without a trauma history, found mainly neutral or slightly negative impact of content warnings, leading the authors to conclude that such warnings are neither meaningfully helpful or harmful [18].
The summary of the 2023 meta-analysis of other studies:
Overall, we found that warnings had no effect on affective responses to negative material or on educational outcomes. However, warnings reliably increased anticipatory affect. Findings on avoidance were mixed, suggesting either that warnings have no effect on engagement with material or that they increased engagement with negative material under specific circumstances. Limitations and implications for policy and therapeutic practice are discussed.
The highlights of the 2018 empirical study:
Trigger warnings increase peoples’ perceived emotional vulnerability to trauma.
Trigger warnings increase peoples’ belief that trauma survivors are vulnerable.
Trigger warnings increase anxiety to written material perceived as harmful.
The 2020 Helping or Harming study acknowledges in its conclusions:
Public arguments regarding trigger warnings have been politically charged, complex, and data-poor. Recent research on trigger warnings can importantly inform or perhaps even settle some of these debates. The research suggests that trigger warnings are unhelpful for trauma survivors, college students, trauma-naïve individuals, and mixed groups of participants (Bellet et al., 2018, 2020; Bridgland et al., 2019; Sanson et al., 2019). Given this consistent conclusion, we find no evidence-based reason for educators, administrators, or clinicians to use trigger warnings.
Whether trigger warnings are explicitly harmful is less clear. We found evidence that trigger warnings increase the narrative centrality of trauma among survivors, which is countertherapeutic (Boals & Murrell, 2016).
…
There are contextual limitations on the research that you will find discussed in each piece. Nevertheless, all the best science we have so far keeps indicating that trigger warnings are not helping, and may be harming. Not just to people who think they’re helping themselves and people they know, or who heed such warnings, but in fact to anyone who reads the warnings. At best, with a negligible effect. This is not to do with any cultural alignments, just what we have tested. I keep rechecking the research base as I’m particularly interested in mental health therapies and theories.
-Wade